98%
921
2 minutes
20
ObjectiveTo examine how domain-switching and user characteristics may predict broad susceptibility to digital deception.BackgroundDespite successful automated filtering techniques, humans remain vulnerable to fraud, losing billions of dollars annually. Many scams are delivered by digitally mediated methods, such as phishing emails or fake social media accounts. However, research typically explores susceptibility to these deceptions independently, making it difficult to draw broad conclusions regarding susceptibility to digital deception.MethodWe recruited a representative sample to investigate how susceptibility to deception may vary across digital domains, particularly when switching between domains (i.e., domain-switching). Participants classified stimuli from five different digital domains (i.e., emails, text messages, news headlines, social media accounts, and voicemails), either randomly (i.e., domain-switching) or in separate blocks, and completed measures of cognitive reflection and digital literacy.ResultsThe results suggest that when users struggle to discriminate between deceptive and legitimate stimuli in one digital deception domain, they are likely to struggle in others. Additionally, the results suggest that while cognitive reflection and digital literacy may help insulate users from deception, domain-switching may generally hinder user performance (i.e., slower responses).ConclusionOverall, individuals appear to be consistently vulnerable to deception across digital domains and this vulnerability can be exacerbated by certain task factors (e.g., domain-switching) and user characteristics (e.g., cognitive reflection and digital literacy).ApplicationTo develop more efficacious interventions that enhance user resiliency, research should consider broad training that incorporates correlates of susceptibility (e.g., cognitive reflection and digital literacy), and more realistic task settings (e.g., domain-switching).
Download full-text PDF |
Source |
---|---|
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00187208251363406 | DOI Listing |
BMC Psychol
September 2025
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts and Humanities, King Abdulaziz University, Jeddah, Saudi Arabia.
Objectives/background: Prior studies have claimed that people engage in compulsive buying in an attempt to deal with stress. Nonetheless, not every stressed person engages in compulsive buying. It is therefore important to investigate the cognitive mechanisms underlying such behavior.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFArch Clin Neuropsychol
September 2025
School of Psychology, Western Sydney University, Sydney, Australia.
Objective: Although traditionally associated with mild head trauma, post-concussive symptoms are commonly reported across both healthy and other clinical populations. Existing research indicates that individuals with depression report high levels of post-concussive symptoms, though the underlying causes of this association remain unknown. The current study aimed to explore potential factors underlying this relationship: specifically, how maladaptive and adaptive self-focused cognitive coping styles, namely, rumination and reflection, respectively, differentially contribute to post-concussive symptoms.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFEncephale
September 2025
Université de Reims Champagne-Ardenne, C2S, Reims, France; Groupe Hospitalier Paul-Guiraud, GHT Psy Sud, Boulogne-Billancourt, France. Electronic address:
Objectives: This systematic review aims to evaluate the impact of cognitive-behavioral and metacognitive therapies on cognitive and clinical insight into early schizophrenia disease.
Methods: A comprehensive literature review was conducted across several databases, including Pubmed, PsycINFO, and Cochrane Library, focusing on studies published between 2006 and May 2024. The studies selected for this review were either randomized controlled trials (RCTs), cross-sectional studies, or exploratory studies.
Numerous large-scale epidemiological studies investigating the trajectory of cognitive recovery after ischemic stroke have presented data suggesting an immediate drop in cognition acutely post-stroke followed by persistent, accelerated decline over time when averaged as a group. We sought to further examine this trend, speculating that the average persistent decline may be a reflection of two subgroups with vastly different prognoses: 1) a minority experiencing decline secondary to neurodegenerative processes like vascular dementia and Alzheimer's disease, and 2) a majority without marked progressive brain atrophy who typically see improvement. Our team thus investigated atrophy's association with language recovery, hypothesizing that declining naming performance in the year after left hemisphere ischemic stroke would be correlated to atrophy of the contralesional hemisphere.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFIntroduction: Subjective cognitive complaints often precede declines in objective measures of cognitive performance. Associations of incipient Alzheimer's disease (AD) neuropathology with subjective cognitive complaints may be detectable earlier than associations with neuropsychological testing among cognitively normal individuals.
Methods: We examined the independent associations of positron emission tomography measures of amyloid beta and tau pathologies with longitudinal subjective complaints and memory among 91 cognitively normal Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging participants using linear mixed effects models.